Levels of Identity Confusion and Attachment among Reared-Together MZ and DZ Twin Pairs
By Jay Joseph, Psy.D.
When twin researchers attempt to assess twins’ environmental similarity, they usually ask questions such as whether, as children, twins shared the same bedroom, attended school together, dressed alike, played together, and so on. Answers to these questions reveal that reared-together MZ twin pairs (monozygotic, identical) grow up experiencing much more similar environments than experienced by same-sex DZ twin pairs (dizygotic, fraternal). Although these questions address some aspects of twins’ environmental similarity, they fail to adequately assess the nature of the attachment, conscious attempts to be alike, and the identity confusion experienced by MZ twin pairs to a far greater degree than DZ pairs.
In 1960, family therapy pioneer and twin
study critic Don Jackson described “the intertwining of [MZ] twin identities,
in the ego fusion that in one sense doubles the ego (because the other is
felt as part of the self) and in another sense halves it (because the self is
felt as part of the other).” According to the psychoanalytically oriented
twin researcher Dorothy Burlingham, “Identical twins when they grow up often
fail to develop into separate human entities” (quoted in Jackson). And in their
field-defining 1960 book Behavior Genetics,
John Fuller and William Thompson recognized that “MZ cotwins model their
behavior upon each other to a greater extent than DZ cotwins.”
The table
I present below provides data from all twin studies I am aware of that published
percentage figures (or enough information to calculate percentages) relating directly
to the levels of identity confusion and attachment that twins experience. We
see that in a 1954 study, British twin researcher James Shields found that
47% of the MZ pairs experienced a “degree of attachment” that was “very close,”
whereas only 15% of the DZs experienced a very close degree of attachment.
Swedish researcher Torsten Husén calculated an “index of attachment” for
twins, and found “a considerable mean difference” between MZ and DZ pairs.
Husén concluded in 1959 that MZ pairs “are much more prone to emphasize the
desire to be alike, to be together, to share the same interests, and to have
a feeling of loyalty.” In 1967, Norwegian twin researcher Einar Kringlen
performed a “global evaluation of twin closeness,” and found that 65% of the MZ
pairs had an “extremely strong level of closeness,” which was true for only
17% of the DZ pairs. And in a 1966 twin study, Helen Koch of the University
of Texas found that “Identical [MZ] co-twins tended to be closer to each
other than fraternals [DZs].”
Some
results in the table are taken from twin researchers’ use and development of zygosity
determination questionnaires, which ask twins to answer questions in order to
distinguish MZ from DZ pairs for research and other purposes. Although there are various methodological
issues in the studies shown in the table, the trend is clear that MZ pairs
experience much higher levels of identity confusion and attachment than
experienced by DZ pairs, which argues strongly against the twin method’s crucial
MZ-DZ “equal environments
assumption” (EEA).
Of the
researchers using or developing zygosity determination questionnaires (marked
by an asterisk), only the Cohen group commented on the irony of needing to
demonstrate the great dissimilarity of MZ and DZ childhood environments as a
method of distinguishing between such pairs. The irony is that the twin
method assumes that these environments are not dissimilar. According to Cohen and colleagues, “The impact of
such repeated confusion on individual twinships, or the effect of these
differences between MZ and DZ twins is not known with certainty. However,
such information must cast doubt upon the assumption of environmental
equivalence [EEA]” (Dibble et al.,1978).
The table
below is adapted from my 2015 book The Trouble with
Twin Studies: A Reassessment of Twin Research in the Social and Behavioral
Sciences, New York: Routledge (pp. 166-167).
© 2015 Jay Joseph. An
earlier version of this table appeared in a 2013 article I published in The
Journal of Mind and Behavior.
I ask
twin researchers and their supporters to explain how the results in this
table are consistent with the claim that the twin method’s MZ-DZ equal
environments assumption is valid.
|
|||
The
Environmental Dissimilarity of Reared-Together MZ and DZ Twin Pairs: Levels
of Identity Confusion and Attachment in Studies that Assessed Such
Levels
|
|||
Study
|
Characteristic of the Twin
Relationship
|
MZ
|
DZ
|
von
Bracken, 1934
|
“Closely
attached”
|
87%
|
21%
|
Wilson,
1934
|
“Never
separated from twin”
|
44%
|
27%
|
Mowrer,
1954
|
“Other
twin as member of family that understands me best”
|
61%
|
24%
|
Mowrer,
1954
|
“Should
be closer to my twin than other siblings”
|
70%
|
44%
|
Shields,
1954
|
“Very
close degree of attachment”
|
47%
|
15%
|
Husén,
1959
|
“Very
keen on always being together”
|
50%
|
25%
|
Cederlöf et al., 1961*
|
“As like as two peas”
|
54%
|
0%
|
Koch,
1966
|
“Sees
likeness between himself and twin”
|
78%
|
54%
|
Nichols
& Bilbro, 1966*
|
“Mistaken
for each other by parents (as children)”
|
27%
|
0%
|
Kringlen,
1967
|
“Identity
confusion in childhood”
|
90%
|
10%
|
Kringlen,
1967
|
“Mistaken
for each other by parents and/or sibs”
|
21%
|
0%
|
Kringlen,
1967
|
“Considered
alike as two drops of water”
|
76%
|
0%
|
Kringlen,
1967
|
“Inseparable
as children to an extreme degree”
|
73%
|
19%
|
Kringlen,
1967
|
“Inseparable
as adults to an extreme degree”
|
18%
|
0%
|
Kringlen,
1967
|
“Brought
up ‘as a unit’”
|
72%
|
19%
|
Kringlen,
1967
|
“Global
evaluation of twin closeness”
|
65%
|
17%
|
Cohen et
al., 1973*
|
“Confused
for each other by mother of father”
|
78%
|
10%
|
Cohen et
al., 1973*
|
“Sometimes
confused by other people in family”
|
94%
|
15%
|
Cohen et
al., 1973*
|
“Hard for
strangers to tell them apart”
|
99%
|
16%
|
Cohen et
al., 1975*
|
“Confused
for each other by mother or father”
|
79%
|
1%
|
Cohen et
al., 1975*
|
“Sometimes
confused by other people in family”
|
93%
|
1%
|
Cohen et
al., 1975*
|
“Hard for
strangers to tell them apart”
|
99%
|
8%
|
Dalgard
& Kringlen, 1976
|
“Extreme
or strong interdependence in childhood”
|
86%
|
36%
|
Dalgard
& Kringlen, 1976
|
“Brought
up as a unit”
|
92%
|
75%
|
Dalgard
& Kringlen, 1976
|
“Extreme
or strong closeness in childhood”
|
86%
|
36%
|
Kasriel
& Eaves, 1976*
|
“Confused
for each other in childhood"
|
98%
|
6%
|
Torgersen,
1979*
|
“As alike
as two peas in a pod”
|
83%
|
1%
|
Torgersen,
1979*
|
“Twins
mixed for each other up as children”
|
71%
|
2%
|
Morris-Yates
et al., 1990
|
“Parental
treatment of twins as two individuals”
|
55%
|
83%
|
·
Sources (same-sex twin pair samples sizes; country): Cederlöf et al., 1961, p. 344 (MZ =
81, DZ = 100; Sweden); Cohen et al., 1973, p. 467 (MZ = 94, DZ = 61; U.S.);
Cohen et al., 1975, p. 1374 (MZ = 181, DZ = 84; U.S.); Dalgard &
Kringlen, 1976, p. 224 (MZ = 49, DZ = 89; Norway); Husén, 1959, p. 141 (MZ =
26, DZ = 24; Sweden); Kasriel & Eaves, 1976, p. 265 (MZ = 94, DZ = 84;
U.K.); Koch, 1966, p. 233 (MZ = 70, DZ = 72; U.S.); Kringlen, 1967, p. 115
(MZ = 75, DZ = 42; Norway); Morris-Yates et al., 1990, p. 323 (MZ = 186, DZ =
157; Australia); Mowrer, 1954, pp. 469-470 (based on “612 twins,” status not
stated; U.S.); Nichols & Bilbro, 1966, p. 270 (MZ = 82, DZ = 41; U.S.);
Shields, 1954, p. 234 (MZ = 36, DZ = 26; U.K.); Torgersen, 1979, p. 228 (MZ =
98, DZ = 117; Norway); von Bracken, 1934, p. 299 (MZ = 23, DZ = 19; Germany);
Wilson, 1934, p. 334 (MZ = 70, DZ = 55; U.S.).
·
MZ = monozygotic twin pairs; DZ = same-sex dizygotic twin pairs.
Includes studies whose authors provided percentage figures for environmental
similarity, or enough information to calculate percentages. Excluded are
studies whose authors provided only correlations or mean scores, or
correlations between twins' environmental similarity and the trait under
study. Excludes questions such as whether twins shared the same bedroom,
attended school together, dressed alike, played together, etc. The Cohen et
al. 1973 and 1975 studies were based on different twin samples.
·
* Studies obtaining information in
the context of using or developing “zygosity determination” questionnaires
designed to distinguish between MZ and DZ pairs.
|
REFERENCES
Cederlöf,
R., Friberg, L., Jonsson, E., & Kaij, L. (1961). The diagnosis of twin zygosity. Acta Genetica et Statistica Medica, 11,
338-362.
Cohen, D. J., Dibble, E., Grawe, J.
M., & Pollin, W. (1973). Separating identical from fraternal twins. Archives of General Psychiatry, 29,
465-469.
Cohen, D. J., Dibble, E., Grawe, J.,
& Pollin, W. (1975). Reliably separating identical from fraternal twins. Archives of General Psychiatry, 32,
1371-1375.
Dalgard, O. S., & Kringlen, E. (1976). A
Norwegian twin study of criminality. British
Journal of Criminology, 16, 213-232.
Dibble,
E., Cohen, D. J., & Grawe, J. M. (1978). “Methodological issues in twin
research: The assumption of environmental equivalence.” In W. Nance (Ed.), Twin research: Psychology and methodology
(pp. 245-251). New York: Alan R. Liss.
Fuller,
J. L., & Thompson, W. R. (1960). Behavior
genetics. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Husén,
T. (1959). Psychological twin research: A
methodological study. Stockholm: Almqvist & Wiksell.
Jackson,
D. D., (1960). “A critique of the literature on the genetics of schizophrenia.”
In D. Jackson (Ed.), The etiology of schizophrenia
(pp. 37-87). New York: Basic Books.
Joseph,
J. (2013). The use of the classical twin method in the social and behavioral
sciences: The fallacy continues. Journal
of Mind and Behavior, 34, 1-39.
Kasriel, J., & Eaves, L. (1976).
The zygosity of twins: Further evidence on the agreement between diagnosis by
blood groups and written questionnaires. Journal
of Biosocial Science, 8, 263-266.
Koch,
H. L. (1966). Twins and twin relations.
Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Kringlen,
E. (1967). Heredity and environment in
the functional psychoses: An epidemiological-clinical study. Oslo:
Universitetsforlaget.
Morris-Yates,
A., Andrews, G., Howie, P., & Henderson, S. (1990). Twins: A test of the
equal environments assumption. Acta
Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 81, 322-326.
Mowrer,
E. R. (1954). Some factors in the affectional adjustment of twins. American Sociological Review, 19, 468–471.
Nichols, R. C., & Bilbro, W. C.
(1966). The diagnosis of twin zygosity. Acta
Genetica et Statistica Medica, 16, 265-275.
Shields,
J. (1954). Personality differences and neurotic traits in normal twin
schoolchildren. Eugenics Review, 45,
213-246.
Torgersen, S. (1979). The
determination of zygosity by means of a mailed questionnaire. Acta Geneticae Medicae et Gemellologiae, 28,
225-236.
von Bracken, H.
(1934). Mutual intimacy
in twins. Character and Personality, 2,
293-309.
Wilson, P. T. (1934). A study of twins
with special reference to heredity as a factor determining differences in
environment. Human Biology, 6,
324-354.
Comments
Post a Comment